Similar to:-  Behavioral and Brain Sciences  (2000) 23:467-8

LOCALIST  REPRESENTATION  CAN  IMPROVE  EFFICIENCY  FOR  DETECTION  AND COUNTING

Commentary  by Horace Barlow* and Anthony Gardner-Medwin† on
CONNECTIONIST MODELLING by Mike Page

*Physiological Laboratory, Cambridge CB2 3EG, England;
e-mail  hbb10@cam.ac.uk
†Department of Physiology, UCL, London WC1E 6BT
e-mail: a.gardner-medwin@ucl.ac.uk

ABSTRACT
Almost all representations have both distributed and localist aspects, depending upon what properties of the data are being considered.  With noisy data, features represented in a localist way can be detected very efficiently, and in binary representations they can be counted more efficiently than those represented in a distributed way.   Brains operate in noisy environments, so the localist representation of behaviourally important events is advantageous, and fits what has been found experi-mentally.  Distributed representations require more neurons to perform as efficiently, but they do have greater versatility.

 As well as the merits Page argues for, localist representations have quantit-ative advantages that he does not bring out.  The brain operates in an uncertain world where important signals are always liable to be contaminated and masked by unwanted ones, so it is important to consider how external noise from the environmen-t affects the reliability and effectiveness of different forms of represen-tation.  In what follows we shall adopt Page's definitions of localist and distributed representation, according to which almost any scheme or model has both compo-nents.  In a scheme emphasising localist representation, the elements can none-the-less be used in combinations, and such combinations represent in a distributed way whatever input events cause them to occur.  Similarly in a scheme emphasising distributed representation, each particular element is activated by a particular subset of the possible input patterns, and it represents this subset in a localist way; for example, a single bit in the ASCII code is a localist representation of the somewhat arbitrary collection of ASCII characters for which it is ON.  We shall also assume for simplicity that the brain represents noisy data, but does not necessarily add noise; of course this is a simplification, but it is the appropriate starting point for the present problem.

Localist representations and matched filters  The principle of a matched filter is to collect all the signal caused by the target that is to be detected, and only this signal, excluding as much as possible signals caused by other stimuli.  In this way the response from the target is maximised while pollution by noise from non-target stimuli is minimised, yielding the best possible signal/noise ratio.  Localist representation-s of features or patterns in the input data can be close approximations to matched filters.  If the representation's elements are linear and use continuous variables, their outputs will be the weighted sums of their different inputs.  If each weight is proportional to the ratio of signal amplitude to noise variance for that part of the input when the desired target is presented, the element will be a matched filter for that target.

 Some neurons in sensory areas of the cortex follow this prescription well, and it makes good sense to regard them as members of a vast array of matched filters, each with slightly different parameters for its trigger feature or optimal stimulus.  In V5 or MT (an area specialising in coherent motion over small regions of the visual field) the receptive fields of the neurons differ from each other in positio-n, size, direction and velocity of their preferred motion (Maunsell & Van Essen, 1983; Felleman & Kaas, 1984; Raiguel et al, 1995), and preferred depth or disparity of the stimulus (Maunsell & Van Essen, 1983; DeAngelis, Cummins & Newsome, 1998).  It has been shown that many individual neurons can detect coherent motion with as great sensitivity as the entire conscious monkey (Newsome, Britten & Movshon, 1989;  Britten, Shadlen, Newsome & Movshon, 1992).  Furthermore human performance in similar tasks varies with stimulus parameters (area, duration, dot density etc) as if it was limited by the noise or uncertainty inherent in the stochastic stimuli that are used, so external noise appears to be an important limit (Barlow, & Tripathy, 1997).  On re-examination it also turns out that external noise is important in monkey MT neurons (Mogi & Barlow, 1998).  For neurons to perform as well as they do, they must have proper-ties close to those of optimum matched filters, which suggests that the whole visual cortex is a localist representation of the visual field using numerous different arrays of filters matched to different classes of feature.  This insight may well apply to all sensory areas of the cortex and even to non-sensory parts, in which case the cortex would be a strongly localist representation throughout.

 Can efficient detection at higher levels always be done by the weighted combination of inputs from the elements of distributed representations at lower levels?  This would require graded signals between the levels, and it is doubtful if those passing between cortical neurons have sufficient dynamic range.  With binary signals, and a task of counting occurrences rather than extracting signals from noise, there is an analogous problem of diminishing the effects of overlap in distributed representations.

 Counting accuracy in localist and distributed representation
 For many of the computations that are important in the brain, such as learning, or detecting that two stimuli are associated, it is necessary to count or estimate how often a specific type of event has occurred.  It is easy to see that, because the elements active in the distributed representation of an event that is to be counted also respond to other events, the mean response rates of those elements will be greater than the mean responses due solely to the event to be counted.  The average effect of this inflation can readily be allowed for, but in a noisy environ-ment the variance as well as the mean will be increased, and this cannot be corrected.  The only way to avoid this problem completely would be to have localist representations for the counted events, though as shown elsewhere (Gardner-Medwin & Barlow, 1999), distributed representations can be efficient at counting if they employ enough elements with sufficient redundancy.

 It may be suggested that brains often learn from a single experience and do not need to count accurately, but such an argument would be misleading.  Efficient statistics are what an animal needs in order to make correct inferences with the minimum amount of data collection, and this is more, not less, important when the number of available trials is low.  A system cannot use inefficient methods of representa-tion if one-shot learning is to occur reliably when it is appropriate and not when it isn’t.

 The relative merits of localist and distributed representations are sometimes finely balanced and are discussed in greater detail elsewhere (Gardner-Medwin & Barlow, 1999).  Localist representations have the edge in terms of efficiency, but one must know in advance what needs to be detected and counted, so they are mainly appropriate for frequent, regularly recurring features of the environment.  In spite of the large numbers of neurons required, the ability of distributed represen-tations to handle unexpected and unprespecified events without ambiguity makes them better for handling novel experiences.

The principle of local computation
 Finally it should be pointed out that the merit of localist representations stems from the fact that computation in the brain is done by local biophysical processes.  Every element of a computation requires a locus in the brain where all the necessary factors are collected together so that they can take part in the bio-physical process.  As an example of the relevance of this principle consider the Hebbian assumption about the locus of learning.  Biophysical processes close to a synapse can readily be influenced by both pre- and post-synaptic activity, since the required information is present there in the way that the principle requires, but it would not be reasonable to assume that distributed patterns of synchronous activity in remote neurons could work in the same way. The implied ban on "action at a distance" may eventually need qualification through better understanding of neuromodulators and dendritic interactions, but localist representations have the advantage that they already collect at one element all the information required for detection and counting; this is what makes it possible for them to perform these jobs efficiently.

 Page ends his manifesto by saying  "...if the brain does not use localist representations then evolution has missed an excellent trick."  Plenty of neuro-physiological evidence shows that it has not, in fact, missed this trick which is so valuable for achieving sensitive and reliable detection of weak signals in a noisy background, and for the frequency estimations needed for reliable learning.  Doubtless evolution has also exploited the advantages that distributed representation can bring to the handling of the unexpected.
 

References

Barlow, H.B. & Tripathy, S.P. (1997)  Correspondence noise and signal pooling as factors determining the detectability of coherent visual motion. Journal of Neuroscience,  17  7954-7966.

Britten, K.H., Shadlen, M.N., Newsome, W.T. & Movshon, J.A. (1992)  The analysis of visual motion: a comparison of neuronal and psychophysical performance. Journal of Neuroscience, 12  4745-4765.

DeAngelis, G.C., Cummins, B.G. & Newsome, W.T. (1998)  Cortical area MT and the perception of stereoscopic depth. Nature (London), 394  677-680.

Felleman, D.J. & Kaas, J.H. (1984)  Receptive field properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area (MT) of owl monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 52  488-513.

Gardner-Medwin, A.R. & Barlow, H.B. (1999)  The limits of counting accuracy in distributed neural representations. Submitted to Neural Computation  19 July 1999,

Maunsell, J.H.R. & Van Essen, D.C. (1983)  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque.  I  Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed and orientation. Journal of Neurophysiology, 49  1127-1147.

Maunsell, J.H.R. & Van Essen, D.C. (1983)  Functional properties of neurons in the middle temporal area of the macaque monkey.  II  Binocular interactions and sensitivity to binocular disparity. Journal of Neurophysiology, 49  1148-1167.

Mogi, K. & Barlow, H.B. (1998)  The variability in neural responses from MT. Journal of Physiology, London, 515  101P-102P.

Newsome, W.T., Britten, K.H. & Movshon, J.A. (1989)  Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature, 341  52-54.

Raiguel, S.,  Van Hulle, M.M.,  Xiao, D.-K.,  Marcar,  V.L. & Orban, G.A. (1995)  Shape and spatial distribution of receptive fields and antagonistic motion surrounds in the middle temporal area (V5) of the macaque. European Journal of Neuroscience, 7  2064-2082.